Formants frequencies variability in French vowels under the effect of various speaking styles
نویسنده
چکیده
The paper presents first results of a research aiming at a more refined definition of the so-called speaking styles. A speaker is recorded in 6 situations and the formants of his lil, /a/ and Iul vowels are studied. New categorizations of the speaking styles are proposed, on the basis of the similarities and dissimilarities in formant frequencies values drawn from the various communication situations. The last five years have been characterized by a constantly increasing interest of phoneticians in the study of speaking styles. Several experiments have shown that changes in communication situations provoke acoustical variations in the speech segments (1-6). Nevertheless, the conceptual framework for the description of speaking styles has remained rather poor. Very often, highly contrasted situations are used, that differ in all respects one from another. For instance, our previous investigations (7-10) have mainly focussed upon two very differenciated styles: the so-called laboratory speech (words reading task), v e r s u s the real spontaneous speech (natural conversation). In this paper, we seek to refine the model for categorization of speech samples. Six contrasted situations will be used, on the occasion of 3 recording sessions. The aim of this exploratory study is to determine whether the concept of re feren t ia l s p e e c h can be supported by some homogeneity in the data drawn from different referential situations, as opposed to l abora tory and spontaneous speech. The experiment focusses on the vowels system dynamics in French. Given the complexity of the experimental procedure, and because of the exploratory nature of the research, we nevertheless restrict our observations to the so-called "cardinal" vowels of French, i.e, the more extreme ones in the system: Iil, la/ and /u/. Moreover, in order to avoid contamination by other surrounding vowels, vowels in CVC structures only are taken into account. 2.Experimental procedure The reported experiment involves a single speaker, recorded on 3 occasions, at oneweek intervals. Four situations were set up during the first session, and one in each of the other two. During the first session, the speaker was asked to describe a cartoon-like picture. This contained no written dialogues or comments; it depicted a very complicated scene where c a . 20 different characters interacted with each other. Each character had very special Article published online by EDP Sciences and available at http://dx.doi.org/10.1051/jp4:19945108 JOURNAL DE PHYSIQUE IV particularities, such a yellow foot, a green tongue, a red bow-hat, i.e. the picture showed very unlikely items. The speaker was seated in a sound-proof room, in front of the picture. The following four tasks were proposed to him in succession: situation I: the experimenter asks the speaker to list the "bizarre details" in the picture; situation 2: the experimenter asks the speaker to make a more comprehensive description of the picture, indicating the relationships between characters and trying to produce a n a r r a t i o n ; situation 3: same task as in situation 2, but the speaker has been given a headphone and from times to times, an unknown person supposed not to know the picture asks for complementary information through the headset; situation 4: the speaker is told that a new person who has never seen the picture will have to reproduce it on the basis of the information he delivers. That person listens to the speaker and frequently asks him for complementary information through the headset. During the second session (situation 5 ) , the speaker had a spontaneous conversation with an experimenter, who asked him to evoke various themes, such his birthplace, military service, or current occupation. The talk lasted for about half an hour. The whole recording was thereafter manually transcripted. Three sets of vowel sounds (at least 25 for each vowel) were randomly selected for analyzis. A list of the words containing the so obtained vowels set was composed. The third session (situation 6) took place a week after the previous one. The same speaker was asked to record all the words of the list, presented in a random order. All sessions took place in the same room, at the Phonetics Laboratory of Mons University. The subject's productions were recorded by means of a Neuman u87i microphone connected to a Sony PCM digitizing unit; the digitized signals were stored on VHS cassettes (Sony video recorder). 3.Acoustical analyzes All the available sounds from the first 4 situations were retained, together with the 90 from situations 5 and 6. At least 25 samples of each phoneme in each situation is therefore available, leading to a total amount of 859 sounds, i.e., 309 [i], 192 [u] and 343 [a]. The first and second formant of each selected vowel were measured by means of a DSP 5500 KAY analyzer, on the basis of wide band spectrograms and narrow band (10 Hz resolution) spectra. Each formant was measured at its center. 4.Statistical analyzes The available data were statistically analyzed vowel by vowel and formant by formant. For a given formant of a given vowel, 6 independent samples of formant values were available (1 set by situation) and underwent one-way analysis of variance. Table 1 summarizes the results drawn from this treatment. Except for F2 in /u/, the data show significant situation-induced overall variabilities of the formants' values. A pairedsituations treatment aimed at locating differences therefore appears fully justified: its results are summarized in table 2 (two-sample Student t-tests). As these tables show, no differences appear ever between situations 1 and 2. On the whole, situations 1 to 4 are rarely differentiated one from another. No significant differences involving second formants are to be observed. When significant differences appear in this subgroup, they always involve at least situation 3 or situation 4. In brief, it seems that the spontaneous speech situation is very different from the others. It appears particularly differentiated from situations 1-4 (picture description), which constitute a rather homogeneous group. It is to be noticed that when differences occur in this group, they involve the two situations where exchanges with the experimenter take place (3 and 4). The words reading task (situation 6) also appears different from the others, i.e. mainly situations 1-4; only two significant differences between situations 5 and 6 are to be observed (F1 of /a/ and F2 of /i/). First formant Second formant F-statistic Significance F-statistic Significance Table 1: F-statistic and significance level of analyses of variance performed on each vowel and each formant. The more numerous significant differences are those involving situation 5 (spontaneous speech); several differences between situation 6 (words reading task) and others can however be observed. first formant second formant Table 2: Results of two-samples between-situations Student t tests performed on each possible pair of situations for the first formant (left-hand matrices) and the second formant (righthand matrices) of vowels /i/, /a/ and /u/. Each star indicates significance at the .05 level. As shown by topologic analyzes of the vowels in the formantic plane, the relationships between the spontaneous speechand the laboratory speech vowels' clusters are similar to those reported in our previous papers (7-lo), i.e., they show a clear tendency to more centralized formant values in spontaneous speech. Generally speaking, the vowels' systems from situations 1-4 show F2 values rather close to those from laboratory speech (few or no significant differences); the first formants' values tend to be lower than those from JOURNAL DE PHYSIQUE IV spontaneousand laboratory speech, as if the system had been shifted towards lower F1 frequency values. Our results show that the 4 situations where the speaker had to describe a picture share common characteristics. Moreover, the spontaneous speech situation appears very well differentiated from other situations. Words reading task also produces specific behaviors of the vowels. It seems also that, broadly speaking, situations where the speaker has to interact frequently with another person tend to show substantial contrast with the other ones. These observations could be explained by the effects of at least two sources of variation: 1. the more or less communicative aspect of the speech produced; 2. the function of the language involved. Very temptatively, those findings could lead to the bases of a model for the classification of the speaking styles. This would consist of two main axes. The first one would express a measure of the magnitude of the communicative intent: reading a list of words, without any real intention to communicate could be at an end of the axis, although interacting frequently with another person could be at the other end. The second axis would be a discrete, non hierarchical one, where speech acts could be classified, e. g., following Jakobson's classification of the language's functions; in the very case of our study, picture description would be typically referential, although spontaneous speech would be expressive. [ I ] Delattre, P. "An acoustic and articulatory study of vowel reduction in four languages", International Review of Applied Linguistics, 7 , (1969), 295-325. [2] Koopmans van Beinum, F, Vowel contrast reduction. An acoustic and perceptual study of Dutch vowels in various speech conditions, (Academische Press, Amsterdam, 1980). [3] Lindblom,B., "Spectrographic study of vowel reduction", J. Acoust. Soc. Am., 3 5 , (1963), 1771-1781. [4] Lobacz , P., "Speech rate and vowel formants", Speech Analyzis and Synthesis, 4,(1976), 186-218. [5] Nord L "Vowel reduction-centralization or contextual assimilation", S p e e c h Communication Seminar, Stockholm, (1974), preprint version. [6] den Os, E , "Vowel reduction in Italian and Dutch", PRIPU, 10,2,(1985) 3-12. [7] Harmegnies, B.; Poch-OlivB, D. , "A study of style-induced vowel variability: Laboratory versus spontaneous speech in Spanish", Speech Communication, 11(1992), 429-437. [8] Poch-OlivB, D.; Harmegnies, B. , "Variations structurelles des systBmes vocaliques en frangais et espagnol sous I'effet du style de parole", Journal de Physique C1, supple'ment au Journal de Physique 111, 2, (1992), 283-286. [9] Blecua, B.; Poch-OlivB, D.; Harmegnies, B, , "Variaciones en la organizacion de las vocales del espaiiol y del catalan en funci6n del estilo de habla", Actas de las I Jornadas Internacionales de Lingiiistica Aplicada, (Publicaciones de la Universidad de Granada), 1 , (1993),. 98-107. [ lo] Delplancq, V.; Harmegnies, B.; Poch-OlivB, D., "Etude de la variabilitk formantique des voyelles du portugais sous 1' effet du style de parole", Sociedade Portuguesa da Lingiiistica, Coimbra, september 1993.
منابع مشابه
بررسی ساختار سازهای واکههای زبان فارسی در بزرگسالان دوزبانه آذری فارسی
Objective: Vowels are the center of syllables while formant structures are one of the most important acoustic characteristics of speech sounds that help in their articulatory and perceptual aspects. Formants represent the shape and size of the vocal tract. There exist trivial differences between the vocal tracts of different people due to which the formant structures of a vowel in one person ar...
متن کاملMeasurement of vowel nasalization by multi - dimensional acoustic analysis
When the velum lowers during vowels, the velopharyngeal port is opened, and there is acoustic coupling between the nasal cavity and the main vocal tract, giving rise to a distinct acoustic quality which we call nasality. Nasal coupling results in energy losses at low frequencies, damping of oral formants (especially F1), and introduction of nasal formants corresponding to the resonances of the ...
متن کاملUsing phonetic constraints in acoustic
The goal of this work is to recover articulatory information from the speech signal by acoustic-to-articulatory inversion. One of the main difficulties with inversion is that the problem is underdetermined and inversion methods generally offer no guarantee on the phonetical realism of the inverse solutions. A way to adress this issue is to use additional phonetic constraints. Knowledge of the p...
متن کاملUsing phonetic constraints in acoustic-to-articulatory inversion
The goal of this work is to recover articulatory information from the speech signal by acoustic-to-articulatory inversion. One of the main difficulties with inversion is that the problem is underdetermined and inversion methods generally offer no guarantee on the phonetical realism of the inverse solutions. A way to adress this issue is to use additional phonetic constraints. Knowledge of the p...
متن کاملMommy is only happy! Dutch mothers' realisation of speech sounds in infant-directed speech expresses emotion, not didactic intent.
Exaggeration of the vowel space in infant-directed speech (IDS) is well documented for English, but not consistently replicated in other languages or for other speech-sound contrasts. A second attested, but less discussed, pattern of change in IDS is an overall rise of the formant frequencies, which may reflect an affective speaking style. The present study investigates longitudinally how Dutch...
متن کاملذخیره در منابع من
با ذخیره ی این منبع در منابع من، دسترسی به آن را برای استفاده های بعدی آسان تر کنید
عنوان ژورنال:
دوره شماره
صفحات -
تاریخ انتشار 2007